Here’s a game of luck:
Play guess the move while watching a couple of 500ish rated players play.
I got 0/12 in one effort. I’m usually lucky to score 50%.
This made me laugh out loud. Pure genius.
Here’s a game of luck:
Play guess the move while watching a couple of 500ish rated players play.
I got 0/12 in one effort. I’m usually lucky to score 50%.
This made me laugh out loud. Pure genius.
Here's a case in point .... a recently finished game 0f Chess960 3 day rated. My opponent was a lot more skillful than I thought he would be and I didn't press hard enough in the opening. I probably had a forced win there. But in the ending, I had a choice of bishop moves and I think I drew because I played the wrong one. It's a difficult ending which is far beyond the scope of the analysis tool to play, so no help there.
39.Kf2 with 40.g3 to follow. I think you get a slight edge, but Black may hold. I agree it is a difficult ending.
No point playing 38 Kf2 instead. His next move is to attack the B so it has to move in any case. The question is where the B moves to. Since he only has one good 38th move, I prefer to move the B first.
My thinking is that the only way to win from that point is to get your king active on the kingside.
I think you were slightly worse when it became a bishop ending with all four bishops, but then your opponent dropped a pawn.
Longing for a walk in the woods.>>>
Visit us, in Wigan. A ten or eleven mile walk takes me through town and through woodland which can't be bettered anywhere, and home again. It's what used to be the estate of the Earls of Crawford, so it's largely unspoiled.
That sounds delightful.
I meant 39 Kf2 of course. Have you seen how I played it and could you improve on it after the B move? The B had to be moved, after all. I thought I was very slightly better with the four bishops on because I could maneuver, since I had slightly more space and indeed, my opponent blundered when only under slight pressure. I had worked out a method of penetrating his position but it wasn't necessary. But did the blunder lose?
I tried my line against Stockfish and reached almost the same position you had in the game, which ends as a draw. Further study, however, showed two moments where your opponent blundered you into a win, and you missed the chance.
44.Be6+- Not so easy to see, perhaps.
62.Kd8+- also wins
I'n not unfamiliar with game theory but you're speaking from a theoretical or ideal standpoint, regarding it. Game theory is, after all, a simplified, hypothetical analysis of game strategy but that isn't entirely useful where the calculations involved to calculate the best moves are often too difficult even for the most powerful computers in the World.
You are unfamiliar. The game theory is not about figuring out what the best decision is. It is not hypothetical or simplified or any sort of analysis of any particular game's strategy. It is a branch of mathematics that studies games in the most fundamental way possible. It does not concern itself with calculations involved in a chess game. But it does analyze games in terms of what kind of factors are important when it comes to making a decision and whether they are in control of the player or not (among many other things).
I had to read a book on game theory in a graduate course in anthropology. For my paper, I offered a deconstructive reading of the text highlighting its failure to recognize the possibility of a draw. I used chess to illustrate. My professor did not know what to do with me.
Truth is it was a crappy paper. But I got a good grade.
The focus of the book attempted to use game theory to understand how humans developed (in an evolutionary sense) altruism.
<<Also there's absolutely no randomness involved.>>
So for practical purposes that's incorrect. It's correct only in an idealised setting where the best decisions CAN be made.
It is correct, period. There's just no randomness in chess. Not being able to make the best decision is not caused by presence of randomness, but by a lack of skill.
Me too. That graduate course in anthropology was 30 years ago. Can't recall the title of the book.
Me too. That graduate course in anthropology was 30 years ago. Can't recall the title of the book.
The selfish gene by Richard Dawkins?
The course, ANTH 510 Advances in Anthropology was taught by four professors, each taking a quarter of the course. We had Geoff Gamble on cognitive linguistics; Grover Krantz on evolution; Tim Kohler on game theory; Fekri Hassan on cognition.
Krantz was notorious because he believed in Sasquatch and even had an ultralight helicopter he used for hunting the unseen beast. Another student asked once, "if you found Bigfoot, what would you do?" Without missing a beat nor lifting his eyes from the center of the table where he always looked (the seminar was held around a large conference table), Krantz said, "put him on the table and dissect him." A true man of science. Krantz became a Bigfoot believer because of a footprint he examined. The footprint exhibited bunions, which he thought couldn't be faked. As I understand, since he died, its been proven that you can.
Me too. That graduate course in anthropology was 30 years ago. Can't recall the title of the book.
The selfish gene by Richard Dawkins?
No. A book on game theory and altruism.
Dawkins was referenced, but no professor would assign in graduate school something everyone was expected to know before they got there.
Me too. That graduate course in anthropology was 30 years ago. Can't recall the title of the book.
The selfish gene by Richard Dawkins?
No. A book on game theory and altruism.
Dawkins was referenced, but no professor would assign in graduate school something everyone was expected to know before they got there.
Sorry, never was an anthropology student
Here’s a game of luck:
Play guess the move while watching a couple of 500ish rated players play.
I got 0/12 in one effort. I’m usually lucky to score 50%.